scholarly article | Q13442814 |
P6179 | Dimensions Publication ID | 1010111010 |
P356 | DOI | 10.3758/BF03196784 |
P698 | PubMed publication ID | 16524009 |
P50 | author | Colin J. Davis | Q49036580 |
P2093 | author name string | Marcus Taff | |
P2860 | cites work | DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud | Q28201828 |
DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy | Q30922278 | ||
N-watch: a program for deriving neighborhood size and other psycholinguistic statistics. | Q38413273 | ||
Interfering neighbours: the impact of novel word learning on the identification of visually similar words. | Q38414818 | ||
Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: a multiple read-out model | Q38459776 | ||
No enemies in the neighborhood: absence of inhibitory neighborhood effects in lexical decision and semantic categorization | Q38460410 | ||
Morphological and orthographic similarity in visual word recognition | Q38462363 | ||
Neighborhood frequency effects and letter visibility in visual word recognition | Q38474481 | ||
On the role of competing word units in visual word recognition: the neighborhood frequency effect | Q38483587 | ||
Neighborhood-frequency effects when primes and targets are of different lengths | Q49074198 | ||
The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. | Q52207869 | ||
The effects of neighborhood frequency in reading and lexical decision. | Q52530951 | ||
Masked priming with graphemically related forms: Repetition or partial activation? | Q56838714 | ||
Orthographic processing in visual word identification | Q68623791 | ||
Neighborhood effects in visual word recognition: effects of letter delay and nonword context difficulty | Q72527290 | ||
No more problems in Coltheart's neighborhood: resolving neighborhood conflicts in the lexical decision task | Q77567117 | ||
P433 | issue | 5 | |
P304 | page(s) | 904-910 | |
P577 | publication date | 2005-10-01 | |
P1433 | published in | Psychonomic Bulletin and Review | Q15763410 |
P1476 | title | More words in the neighborhood: interference in lexical decision due to deletion neighbors | |
P478 | volume | 12 |
Q90089995 | A Word on Words in Words: How Do Embedded Words Affect Reading? |
Q48450102 | BuscaPalabras: a program for deriving orthographic and phonological neighborhood statistics and other psycholinguistic indices in Spanish. |
Q38388628 | Children like dense neighborhoods: Orthographic neighborhood density effects in novel readers |
Q38373049 | Does LGHT prime DARK? Masked associative priming with addition neighbors |
Q51918724 | E-Hitz: a word frequency list and a program for deriving psycholinguistic statistics in an agglutinative language (Basque). |
Q51871352 | GreekLex: a lexical database of Modern Greek. |
Q38384210 | Inhibitory neighbor priming effects in eye movements during reading. |
Q38431353 | Inhibitory stroke neighbour priming in character recognition and reading in Chinese |
Q36161714 | Lexical embeddings produce interference when they are morphologically unrelated to the words in which they are contained: Evidence from eye movements |
Q93205817 | Little evidence for Fast Mapping (FM) in adults: A review and discussion |
Q38135066 | Models of visual word recognition. |
Q30477328 | Mrs. Malaprop's Neighborhood: Using Word Errors to Reveal Neighborhood Structure |
Q38376147 | Orthographic and associative neighborhood density effects: what is shared, what is different? |
Q36200479 | Orthographic neighborhood effects as a function of word frequency: an event-related potential study |
Q60512027 | Taking the Book from the Bookshelf: Masked Constituent Priming Effects from Compound Words and Nonwords |
Q33579845 | The French Lexicon Project: lexical decision data for 38,840 French words and 38,840 pseudowords |
Q48665995 | There is no clam with coats in the calm coast: delimiting the transposed-letter priming effect |
Q49160676 | Who is dominating the Dutch neighbourhood? On the role of subsyllabic units in Dutch nonword reading. |
Search more.