Misinformation and Its Correction: Continued Influence and Successful Debiasing

scientific article published on December 2012

Misinformation and Its Correction: Continued Influence and Successful Debiasing is …
instance of (P31):
scholarly articleQ13442814

External links are
P356DOI10.1177/1529100612451018
P698PubMed publication ID26173286
P5875ResearchGate publication ID258180567

P50authorNorbert SchwarzQ945656
Stephan LewandowskyQ16192820
John CookQ31211330
Ullrich K. H. EckerQ58141983
Colleen M. SeifertQ42305957
P2860cites workWhy the "Death Panel" Myth Wouldn't Die: Misinformation in the Health Care Reform DebateQ22241311
The Age-Old Struggle against the AntivaccinationistsQ22248117
Expert credibility in climate changeQ24614933
Political conservatism as motivated social cognitionQ28208498
Source monitoringQ28261181
Examining the Scientific Consensus on Climate ChangeQ28314707
Dissemination of health information through social networks: twitter and antibioticsQ28709660
Public health and medicine in an age of energy scarcity: the case of petroleumQ28741580
Polarization and Partisan Selective ExposureQ29028177
The Elaboration Likelihood Model of PersuasionQ29042061
Balance as bias: global warming and the US prestige pressQ29305422
Simplicity: a unifying principle in cognitive science?Q30332380
YouTube as a source of information on the H1N1 influenza pandemic.Q30385861
Mental contamination and mental correction: unwanted influences on judgments and evaluationsQ30466232
Talking about others: Emotionality and the dissemination of social informationQ57916272
Framing peak petroleum as a public health problem: audience research and participatory engagement in the United StatesQ58066229
The Boomerang Effect A Synthesis of Findings and a Preliminary Theoretical FrameworkQ58276256
Dissociation of processes in belief: Source recollection, statement familiarity, and the illusion of truthQ59664285
Fluency and the Detection of Misleading Questions: Low Processing Fluency Attenuates the Moses IllusionQ60517591
Metacognitive Experiences and Human JudgmentQ60517679
Metacognitive Experiences in Consumer Judgment and Decision MakingQ60517733
Judgment in a Social Context: Biases, Shortcomings, and the Logic of ConversationQ60517935
Misinformation and the Currency of Democratic CitizenshipQ61128684
Reducing misinformation effects in children with cognitive interviews: dissociating recollection and familiarityQ73499449
Slimming on the InternetQ73965044
Intended and unintended effects of explicit warnings on eyewitness suggestibility: evidence from source identification testsQ77820875
Boosting wisdom: distance from the self enhances wise reasoning, attitudes, and behaviorQ84505608
Birds of a feather flock conjointly (?): rhyme as reason in aphorismsQ33422913
System justification, the denial of global warming, and the possibility of "system-sanctioned change".Q33518344
Genetically modified myths and realitiesQ33627107
Biased but in doubt: conflict and decision confidence.Q33808679
Effects of perceptual fluency on judgments of truthQ33874199
Health information on the Internet: accessibility, quality, and readability in English and SpanishQ33947688
Misinformation effects in eyewitness memory: The presence and absence of memory impairment as a function of warning and misinformation accessibilityQ33972833
Planting misinformation in the human mind: a 30-year investigation of the malleability of memoryQ33988735
The automatic conservative: ideology-based attentional asymmetries in the processing of valenced informationQ34077657
A dirty word or a dirty world?: Attribute framing, political affiliation, and query theoryQ34112460
Models of ecological rationality: the recognition heuristicQ34115766
Addressing the vaccine confidence gap.Q34191703
Medicine. Do defaults save lives?Q34278249
Should medical historians be working for the tobacco industry?Q34313034
Strategic retrieval and the frontal lobes: evidence from confabulation and amnesiaQ34432900
Ironic effects of repetition: measuring age-related differences in memoryQ34491457
Inferring the popularity of an opinion from its familiarity: a repetitive voice can sound like a chorusQ34626019
Could it happen here? Vaccine risk controversies and the specter of derailmentQ36123254
The structure and function of explanationsQ36581851
The gap between science and perception: the case of plant biotechnology in EuropeQ36830764
Second dose of measles, mumps, and rubella vaccine: questionnaire survey of health professionalsQ37137964
Arguing to learn in science: the role of collaborative, critical discourseQ37736114
Transfer of learning in avoiding false memory: the roles of warning, immediate feedback, and incentiveQ38397183
Suspicious spirits, flexible minds: when distrust enhances creativityQ38488128
Emotional selection in memes: the case of urban legendsQ39588726
Influence of emotional content and perceived relevance on spread of urban legends: a pilot studyQ39850391
Terrorists brought down the plane!--No, actually it was a technical fault: processing corrections of emotive information.Q41986623
Ironic effects of drawing attention to story errorsQ42722972
The association of knowledge with concern about global warming: trusted information sources shape public thinkingQ43544322
Correcting over 50 years of tobacco industry misinformationQ44439185
Simplicity and probability in causal explanationQ44600638
The role of group orientation and descriptive norms on water conservation attitudes and behaviorsQ46530350
Reading is believing: the truth effect and source credibilityQ47260033
Believe it or not: on the possibility of suspending beliefQ47263999
Memory for fact, fiction, and misinformation: the Iraq War 2003.Q47290014
Evolving informational credentials: the (mis)attribution of believable facts to credible sourcesQ47349312
Fixing the communications failureQ47413317
Bridging the partisan divide: Self-affirmation reduces ideological closed-mindedness and inflexibility in negotiationQ47758949
Learning errors from fiction: difficulties in reducing reliance on fictional storiesQ47831728
Reducing intergroup prejudice and conflict using the media: a field experiment in RwandaQ48294490
Psychological motives and political orientation--the left, the right, and the rigid: comment on Jost et al. (2003).Q48593008
Setting the record straight: vaccines, autism, and the LancetQ50304030
Fear, misinformation, and innumerates: how the Wakefield paper, the press, and advocacy groups damaged the public healthQ50304806
Arousal increases social transmission of information.Q50623152
An emotion-based model of risk perception and stigma susceptibility: cognitive appraisals of emotion, affective reactivity, worldviews, and risk perceptions in the generation of technological stigma.Q50985914
Correcting false information in memory: manipulating the strength of misinformation encoding and its retraction.Q51018364
Explicit warnings reduce but do not eliminate the continued influence of misinformation.Q51027240
Sponsorship, ambushing, and counter-strategy: effects upon memory for sponsor and event.Q51060707
YouTube as a source of information on immunization: a content analysis.Q51478309
A case for clarity in the writing of health statements.Q51892117
Memory for temporal context: effects of ageing, encoding instructions, and retrieval strategies.Q51932881
Detrimental influence of contextual change on spacing effects in free recall.Q51939409
Revising what readers know: updating text representations during narrative comprehension.Q51964555
Updating accounts following a correction of misinformation.Q51989286
You can't not believe everything you read.Q52034392
When debiasing backfires: accessible content and accessibility experiences in debiasing hindsight.Q52119779
The polarizing effect of news media messages about the social determinants of health.Q53091799
On playing the Nazi card.Q53129643
Perseverance of social theories: The role of explanation in the persistence of discredited informationQ54295944
When Corrections Fail: The Persistence of Political MisperceptionsQ54295966
Motivated Skepticism in the Evaluation of Political BeliefsQ54296022
Biased assimilation and attitude polarization: The effects of prior theories on subsequently considered evidenceQ54296085
A Theory of Social Comparison ProcessesQ55870772
The organisation of denial: Conservative think tanks and environmental scepticismQ55871436
Astroturfing Global Warming: It Isn’t Always Greener on the Other Side of the FenceQ55891498
A novel view of global warmingQ55897412
Sources of the continued influence effect: When misinformation in memory affects later inferencesQ55921740
A critical review of the successful CFC phase-out versus the delayed methyl bromide phase-out in the Montreal ProtocolQ56080606
Education, politics and opinions about climate change evidence for interaction effectsQ56270306
How Warnings about False Claims Become RecommendationsQ56339180
Metacognitive Experiences and the Intricacies of Setting People Straight: Implications for Debiasing and Public Information CampaignsQ56339183
On the binary quality of recognition and the inconsequentiality of further knowledge: two critical tests of the recognition heuristicQ56444146
The value of distrustQ56504975
‘Careless pork costs lives’: Risk stories from science to press release to mediaQ56532422
Scientists and the media: the struggle for legitimacy in climate change and conservation scienceQ56608772
Misperceptions, the Media, and the Iraq WarQ56610419
Anti-reflexivityQ56627322
Frequency and the conference of referential validityQ56698857
Agnotology as a Teaching Tool: Learning Climate Science by Studying MisinformationQ56777324
Misinformation and the “War on Terror”: when memory turns fiction into factQ57731987
P433issue3
P921main subjectmisinformation correctionQ105047006
P304page(s)106-131
P577publication date2012-12-01
P1433published inPsychological Science in the Public InterestQ7256368
P1476titleMisinformation and Its Correction: Continued Influence and Successful Debiasing
P478volume13