Conflicts as aversive signals

scientific article published on 2 January 2012

Conflicts as aversive signals is …
instance of (P31):
scholarly articleQ13442814

External links are
P356DOI10.1016/J.BANDC.2011.12.003
P8608Fatcat IDrelease_af2x3wgmjzfbvgahjiqudpfmla
P698PubMed publication ID22218295
P5875ResearchGate publication ID51983030

P2093author name stringRico Fischer
Gesine Dreisbach
P433issue2
P304page(s)94-98
P577publication date2012-01-02
P1433published inBrain and CognitionQ4955810
P1476titleConflicts as aversive signals
P478volume78

Reverse relations

cites work (P2860)
Q3065276710 years of BAWLing into affective and aesthetic processes in reading: what are the echoes?
Q47921801A closer look at the relationship between the default network, mind wandering, negative mood, and depression
Q91686378Affective Influence on Context-Specific Proportion Congruent (CSPC) Effect
Q93346304Affective influences without approach-avoidance actions: on the congruence between valence and stimulus-response mappings
Q47351836Anticipating cognitive effort: roles of perceived error-likelihood and time demands
Q42083200Anxiety and error monitoring: the importance of motivation and emotion
Q47669215Asymmetric transfer effects between cognitive and affective task disturbances.
Q47203743Attentional adjustment to conflict strength: evidence from the effects of manipulating flanker-target SOA on response times and prestimulus pupil size.
Q47798988Blaming the victims of your own mistakes: How visual search accuracy influences evaluation of stimuli.
Q38067645Blinking predicts enhanced cognitive control
Q90704405Common mechanisms in error monitoring and action effect monitoring
Q49078369Competition elicits arousal and affect
Q47713431Conflict acts as an implicit cost in reinforcement learning
Q47385624Conflict and disfluency as aversive signals: context-specific processing adjustments are modulated by affective location associations.
Q88427345Conflict modification: predictable production of congruent situations facilitates responding in a stroop task
Q47701658Conflict: run! Reduced Stroop interference with avoidance responses
Q38457837Conflicts as aversive signals: conflict priming increases negative judgments for neutral stimuli
Q37142388Congruency sequence effects are driven by previous-trial congruency, not previous-trial response conflict
Q33897365Congruency sequence effects without feature integration or contingency learning confounds.
Q93147114Cost-benefit trade-offs in decision-making and learning
Q34462489Creatures of habit (and control): a multi-level learning perspective on the modulation of congruency effects
Q36135595Different Stages, Different Signals: The Modulating Effect of Cognitive Conflict on Subsequent Processing
Q36595342Dopamine Does Double Duty in Motivating Cognitive Effort.
Q47319904Effects of Intranasal Oxytocin on the Blood Oxygenation Level-Dependent Signal in Food Motivation and Cognitive Control Pathways in Overweight and Obese Men.
Q35143291Emotional foundations of cognitive control
Q92921273Expected Value of Control and the Motivational Control of Habitual Action
Q34422196Frontal midline theta reflects anxiety and cognitive control: meta-analytic evidence
Q48165754Frontal theta accounts for individual differences in the cost of conflict on decision making
Q47846224Going, going, gone? Proactive control prevents the congruency sequence effect from rapid decay
Q49721426Heart work after errors: Behavioral adjustment following error commission involves cardiac effort.
Q47796431History effects in visual search for monsters: search times, choice biases, and liking.
Q58227494Ignorance reflects preference: the influence of selective ignoring on evaluative conditioning
Q47588383Implicit happiness and sadness are associated with ease and difficulty: evidence from sequential priming.
Q34804673In Your Face: Risk of Punishment Enhances Cognitive Control and Error-Related Activity in the Corrugator Supercilii Muscle.
Q91235639Individual Differences in Uncertainty Tolerance Are not Associated With Cognitive Control Functions in the Flanker Task
Q47720802Individual Differences in the Context-Dependent Recruitment of Cognitive Control: Evidence From Action Versus State Orientation
Q46134194Interactions Among Working Memory, Reinforcement Learning, and Effort in Value-Based Choice: A New Paradigm and Selective Deficits in Schizophrenia
Q47378770Matching between oral inward-outward movements of object names and oral movements associated with denoted objects.
Q49144882Metacognition and cognitive control: behavioural adaptation requires conflict experience
Q59656926Monitor yourself! Deficient error-related brain activity predicts real-life self-control failures
Q57174041Monitoring and control in multitasking
Q47742324Mood states influence cognitive control: the case of conflict adaptation
Q47732847No pain, no gain: the affective valence of congruency conditions changes following a successful response
Q90726120Performance feedback promotes proactive but not reactive adaptation of conflict-control
Q47329661Pubertal development and anxiety risk independently relate to startle habituation during fear conditioning in 8-14 year-old females
Q21132511Punishment sensitivity predicts the impact of punishment on cognitive control
Q36881882Pupil dilation in the Simon task as a marker of conflict processing.
Q91485039RETRACTED: Auditory (dis-)fluency triggers sequential processing adjustments
Q33582344Responses of medial and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex to interpersonal conflict for resources
Q90247989Reward Modulates Affective Priming Effect in Cognitive Conflict Processing: Electrophysiological Evidence
Q38377706Rule-violations sensitise towards negative and authority-related stimuli.
Q91584406Social evaluations under conflict: negative judgments of conflicting information are easier than positive judgments
Q47655496Stimulus conflict triggers behavioral avoidance
Q41824507Subjective experience of difficulty depends on multiple cues
Q64891176The Effort Paradox: Effort Is Both Costly and Valued.
Q92622800The Instructed Task-Switch Evaluation Effect: Is the Instruction to Switch Tasks Sufficient to Dislike Task Switch Cues?
Q47397124The Role of Anterior Cingulate Cortex in the Affective Evaluation of Conflict.
Q89522377The effects of induced and trait anxiety on the sequential modulation of emotional conflict
Q92597067The face of control: Corrugator supercilii tracks aversive conflict signals in the service of adaptive cognitive control
Q26824223The heterogeneous world of congruency sequence effects: an update
Q35124157The influence of negative stimulus features on conflict adaption: evidence from fluency of processing
Q39909173The neural networks of subjectively evaluated emotional conflicts
Q42436253The role of affect and reward in the conflict-triggered adjustment of cognitive control
Q92960130The role of the opioid system in decision making and cognitive control: A review
Q36059639The sweetest thing: the influence of angularity, symmetry, and the number of elements on shape-valence and shape-taste matches
Q50631377The time course of the aversive conflict signal.
Q49663000Too Tasty to Be Ignored
Q53402385Transcutaneous vagus nerve stimulation (tVNS) enhances conflict-triggered adjustment of cognitive control.
Q47763906Trauma exposure is associated with increased context-dependent adjustments of cognitive control in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder and healthy controls
Q40446714Uncertainty-dependent activity within the ventral striatum predicts task-related changes in response strategy.
Q47324372What does cognitive control feel like? Effective and ineffective cognitive control is associated with divergent phenomenology
Q64941902When conflict influences liking: The case of the Stroop task.

Search more.