scholarly article | Q13442814 |
P356 | DOI | 10.1037/0096-1523.20.5.1000 |
P698 | PubMed publication ID | 7964526 |
P50 | author | Steven A. Hillyard | Q104720472 |
P2093 | author name string | Luck SJ | |
P433 | issue | 5 | |
P921 | main subject | electrophysiology | Q1154774 |
P304 | page(s) | 1000-1014 | |
P577 | publication date | 1994-10-01 | |
P1433 | published in | Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | Q6295188 |
P1476 | title | Spatial filtering during visual search: evidence from human electrophysiology | |
P478 | volume | 20 |
Q30447581 | A brief introduction to the use of event-related potentials in studies of perception and attention |
Q36370205 | A common neural mechanism for preventing and terminating the allocation of attention |
Q37378627 | A cuing study of the N2pc component: an index of attentional deployment to objects rather than spatial locations |
Q36296934 | Action intentions modulate allocation of visual attention: electrophysiological evidence |
Q37353763 | Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention |
Q35484593 | Active suppression of distractors that match the contents of visual working memory |
Q35117343 | Age and apolipoprotein E ε4 effects on neural correlates of odor memory |
Q92138770 | An EEG study of detection without localisation in change blindness |
Q48627271 | An ERP study of visual spatial attention and letter target detection for isoluminant and nonisoluminant stimuli |
Q91711144 | An Electrophysiological Dissociation of Encoding vs. Maintenance Failures in Visual-Spatial Working Memory |
Q33814506 | An electrophysiological measure of access to representations in visual working memory |
Q48154267 | Anterior N2 predicts subsequent viewing time and interest rating for novel drawings |
Q91817100 | Anticipatory alpha oscillation predicts attentional selection and hemodynamic response |
Q87133396 | Asymmetry of automatic change detection shown by the visual mismatch negativity: an additional feature is identified faster than missing features |
Q48329996 | Attention and movement-related motor cortex activation: a high-density EEG study of spatial stimulus-response compatibility |
Q76383404 | Attention to features precedes attention to locations in visual search: evidence from electromagnetic brain responses in humans |
Q36223762 | Attentional Selection Accompanied by Eye Vergence as Revealed by Event-Related Brain Potentials. |
Q50602584 | Attentional and anatomical considerations for the representation of simple stimuli in visual short-term memory: evidence from human electrophysiology |
Q47908322 | Attentional and intentional cueing in a Simon task: An EEG-based approach |
Q42736167 | Attentional biases in children of depressed mothers: An event-related potential (ERP) study |
Q33594217 | Attentional capture and inhibition of saccades after irrelevant and relevant cues |
Q36643244 | Attentional capture by task-irrelevant fearful faces is revealed by the N2pc component. |
Q37069420 | Attentional capture by visual singletons is mediated by top-down task set: new evidence from the N2pc component. |
Q33681235 | Attentional selection predicts rapid automatized naming ability in Chinese-speaking children with ADHD |
Q33732631 | Attentional spreading to task-irrelevant object features: experimental support and a 3-step model of attention for object-based selection and feature-based processing modulation |
Q30432593 | Auditory event-related potentials associated with perceptual reversals of bistable pitch motion |
Q48888080 | Automaticity and attention: investigating automatic processing in texture segmentation with event-related brain potentials |
Q61803855 | Awareness of Emotional Expressions in Cannabis Users: An Event-Related Potential Study |
Q91591362 | Between one event and two: The locus of the effect of stimulus contrast on temporal integration |
Q48827468 | Biasing perception by spatial long-term memory. |
Q36625608 | Both memory and attention systems contribute to visual search for targets cued by implicitly learned context |
Q89862036 | Brain Processing of Complex Geometric Forms in a Visual Memory Task Increases P2 Amplitude |
Q37346822 | CNTRICS final task selection: control of attention |
Q36995653 | Capture versus suppression of attention by salient singletons: Electrophysiological evidence for an automatic attend-to-me signal |
Q34627368 | Category-based inhibition of focused attention across consecutive trials |
Q47117080 | Changes of Attention during Value-Based Reversal Learning Are Tracked by N2pc and Feedback-Related Negativity |
Q37518487 | Cognitive control activity is modulated by the magnitude of interference and pre-activation of monitoring mechanisms |
Q64978964 | Combined Electrophysiological and Behavioral Evidence for the Suppression of Salient Distractors. |
Q90010171 | Concurrent evaluation of independently cued features during perceptual decisions and saccadic targeting in visual search |
Q47193528 | Contingent orienting or contingent capture: a size singleton matching the target-distractor size relation cannot capture attention. |
Q50140701 | Contralateral Delay Activity Tracks Fluctuations in Working Memory Performance. |
Q33783712 | Contralateral delay activity provides a neural measure of the number of representations in visual working memory |
Q50759800 | Control mechanisms mediating shifts of attention in auditory and visual space: a spatio-temporal ERP analysis |
Q48207980 | Control over response priming in visuomotor processing: a lateralized event-related potential study. |
Q52565535 | Cortical mechanisms of prioritizing selection for rejection in visual search. |
Q63982534 | Cortical sources of the early components of the visual evoked potential |
Q36470210 | Cortico-cortical interactions in spatial attention: A combined ERP/TMS study. |
Q89700489 | Covert Spatial Attention Speeds Target Individuation |
Q30571502 | Decoding the Locus of Covert Visuospatial Attention from EEG Signals |
Q81488050 | Detecting the presence of a singleton involves focal attention |
Q92602582 | Differences in early and late pattern-onset visual-evoked potentials between self- reported migraineurs and controls |
Q34304767 | Direct neurophysiological evidence for spatial suppression surrounding the focus of attention in vision |
Q28601113 | Distinct Mechanisms for Distractor Suppression and Target Facilitation |
Q37427092 | Does focused endogenous attention prevent attentional capture in pop-out visual search? |
Q36070710 | Dynamic weighting of feature dimensions in visual search: behavioral and psychophysiological evidence |
Q58763376 | Dynamics of neural representations when searching for exemplars and categories of human and non-human faces |
Q36060773 | EEG correlates of Fitts's law during preparation for action. |
Q41988742 | ERP markers of target selection discriminate children with high vs. low working memory capacity |
Q30570455 | Early and late selection modulate competition for representation: Evidence from the N2pc in a multiple frame procedure |
Q44492120 | Early information processing contributions to object individuation revealed by perception of illusory figures. |
Q52095727 | Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events. |
Q45986433 | Early processing of orthographic language membership information in bilingual visual word recognition: Evidence from ERPs. |
Q41845145 | Early selection versus late correction: Age-related differences in controlling working memory contents |
Q93003141 | Early spatial attention deployment toward and away from aggressive voices |
Q55278689 | Effects of Mild Cognitive Impairment on the Event-Related Brain Potential Components Elicited in Executive Control Tasks. |
Q48468118 | Effects of pointing direction and direction predictability on event-related lateralizations of the EEG. |
Q78476780 | Effects of sustained, voluntary attention on amplitude and latency of steady-state visual evoked potential: a costs and benefits analysis |
Q36678735 | Efficient attentional selection predicts distractor devaluation: event-related potential evidence for a direct link between attention and emotion |
Q60047156 | Electrophysiological Correlates of the Effect of Task Difficulty on Inhibition of Return |
Q39982943 | Electrophysiological Evidence of Atypical Spatial Attention in Those with a High Level of Self-reported Autistic Traits. |
Q27317207 | Electrophysiological Evidence of a Delay in the Visual Recognition Process in Young Children. |
Q38665800 | Electrophysiological and behavioral evidence reveals the effects of trait anxiety on contingent attentional capture |
Q35067544 | Electrophysiological correlates of amnestic mild cognitive impairment in a simon task. |
Q36646292 | Electrophysiological correlates of change detection. |
Q48115479 | Electrophysiological correlates of stimulus processing in change blindness |
Q92132750 | Electrophysiological correlates of the flexible allocation of visual working memory resources |
Q36184533 | Electrophysiological evidence for adult age-related sparing and decrements in emotion perception and attention |
Q48311180 | Electrophysiological evidence for cognitive control during conflict processing in visual spatial attention |
Q47362244 | Electrophysiological evidence for enhanced attentional deployment in spatial learners |
Q35705260 | Electrophysiological evidence for failures of item individuation in crowded visual displays |
Q43681175 | Electrophysiological evidence for greater attention to threat when cognitive control resources are depleted |
Q48442652 | Electrophysiological evidence for inhibition of return effect in exogenous orienting |
Q53414718 | Electrophysiological evidence for temporal dynamics associated with attentional processing in the zoom lens paradigm. |
Q48147593 | Electrophysiological evidence of central interference in the control of visuospatial attention |
Q59070140 | Electrophysiological measurement of rapid shifts of attention during visual search |
Q47866258 | Emotion-induced blindness reflects competition at early and late processing stages: an ERP study. |
Q47381380 | Emotional faces influence numerosity estimation without awareness |
Q38862462 | Endogenous attention modulates early selective attention in psychopathy: An ERP investigation |
Q58556527 | Estimating the Timing of Cognitive Operations With MEG/EEG Latency Measures: A Primer, a Brief Tutorial, and an Implementation of Various Methods |
Q48785896 | Event-related potentials suggest slowing of brain processes in generalized epilepsy and alterations of visual processing in patients with partial seizures |
Q33667617 | Evidence for a Priori Existence of Attentional Bias Subgroups in Emotional Processing of Aversive Stimuli |
Q35823777 | Explicit behavioral detection of visual changes develops without their implicit neurophysiological detectability. |
Q55003804 | Exploring the impact of chronic high-altitude exposure on visual spatial attention using the ERP approach. |
Q33393635 | Express attentional re-engagement but delayed entry into consciousness following invalid spatial cues in visual search |
Q36294131 | Facilitation or disengagement? Attention bias in facial affect processing after short-term violent video game exposure |
Q47103995 | Fear boosts the early neural coding of faces |
Q64057342 | Flexibility in Attentional Control: Multiple Sources and Suppression |
Q41960251 | From Capture to Inhibition: How does Irrelevant Information Influence Visual Search? Evidence from a Spatial Cuing Paradigm |
Q47849197 | Further Evidence That N2pc Reflects Target Enhancement Rather Than Distracter Suppression |
Q40982153 | Gestalt Perceptual Organization of Visual Stimuli Captures Attention Automatically: Electrophysiological Evidence |
Q37164627 | Global attention facilitates the planning, but not execution of goal-directed reaches |
Q37428872 | Goal-driven attentional capture by invisible colors: evidence from event-related potentials. |
Q50974589 | Habitual action video game playing is associated with caudate nucleus-dependent navigational strategies |
Q53668135 | High resolution EEG indicators of pain responses in relation to hypnotic susceptibility and suggestion. |
Q88567724 | High-precision voluntary movements are largely independent of preceding vertex potentials elicited by sudden sensory events |
Q36108901 | How Attention Changes in Response to Incentives |
Q48446244 | How does attention attenuate target-distractor interference in vision?. Evidence from magnetoencephalographic recordings |
Q39079214 | How to get statistically significant effects in any ERP experiment (and why you shouldn't). |
Q46461464 | Hyperfocusing of attention on goal-related information in schizophrenia: Evidence from electrophysiology |
Q90379475 | Hyperscanning: A Valid Method to Study Neural Inter-brain Underpinnings of Social Interaction |
Q35236894 | Improvement in visual search with practice: mapping learning-related changes in neurocognitive stages of processing |
Q36770670 | Inability to suppress salient distractors predicts low visual working memory capacity |
Q37479599 | Individuation of objects and object parts rely on the same neuronal mechanism |
Q30492004 | Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review |
Q35575381 | Inhibition deficit in the spatial tendency of the response in multiple-domain amnestic mild cognitive impairment. An event-related potential study |
Q48501738 | Intentional pre-cueing does not influence the Simon effect |
Q36929290 | Involuntary attentional capture is determined by task set: evidence from event-related brain potentials |
Q51966398 | Involvement of memory-comparison-based change detection in visual distraction. |
Q41913947 | Lateralization of spatial information processing in response monitoring |
Q51981373 | Lateralized ERP components related to spatial orienting: discriminating the direction of attention from processing sensory aspects of the cue. |
Q90316226 | Lateralized Suppression of Alpha-Band EEG Activity As a Mechanism of Target Processing |
Q36064364 | Linking brainwaves to the brain: an ERP primer. |
Q51992579 | Localization of temporal preparation effects via trisected reaction time. |
Q64069227 | Long-term exposure to high altitude attenuates verbal and spatial working memory: Evidence from an event-related potential study |
Q34124332 | Long-term memories bias sensitivity and target selection in complex scenes |
Q34207180 | Masked targets trigger event-related potentials indexing shifts of attention but not error detection |
Q57203598 | Measurement of ERP latency differences: A comparison of single-participant and jackknife-based scoring methods |
Q81137850 | Mechanisms underlying spatial coding in a multiple-item Simon task |
Q54960246 | Meditation Effects on the Control of Involuntary Contingent Reorienting Revealed With Electroencephalographic and Behavioral Evidence. |
Q101574133 | Memory-driven capture occurs for individual features of an object |
Q48248215 | Modulation of neural activities by enhanced local selection in the processing of compound stimuli |
Q50060277 | More insight into the interplay of response selection and visual attention in dual-tasks: masked visual search and response selection are performed in parallel |
Q41936653 | Multiple Electrophysiological Markers of Visual-Attentional Processing in a Novel Task Directed toward Clinical Use. |
Q30473389 | N2ac: an ERP component associated with the focusing of attention within an auditory scene |
Q37614507 | Naso-Temporal Asymmetries: Suppression of Emotional Faces in the Temporal Visual Hemifield |
Q35560269 | Nasotemporal ERP differences: evidence for increased inhibition of temporal distractors |
Q58754479 | Neural Correlates of Conscious Motion Perception |
Q34379599 | Neural Correlates of Correct and Errant Attentional Selection Revealed Through N2pc and Frontal Eye Field Activity |
Q92782495 | Neural Dynamics of Cognitive Control over Working Memory Capture of Attention |
Q59800849 | Neural Processing of Repeated Search Targets Depends Upon the Stimuli: Real World Stimuli Engage Semantic Processing and Recognition Memory |
Q47597140 | Neural Signature of Value-Based Sensorimotor Prioritization in Humans. |
Q33372134 | Neural correlate of filtering of irrelevant information from visual working memory |
Q47987940 | Neural correlates of inefficient filtering of emotionally neutral distractors from working memory in trait anxiety |
Q36943543 | Neural measures of individual differences in selecting and tracking multiple moving objects |
Q36019175 | Neural measures reveal a fixed item limit in subitizing |
Q33889231 | Neural mechanisms of human perceptual learning: electrophysiological evidence for a two-stage process |
Q53082950 | Neural mechanisms of surround attenuation and distractor competition in visual search. |
Q30571355 | Neural processing stages during object-substitution masking and their relationship to perceptual awareness |
Q82634852 | Neural representation of feature synergy |
Q38422097 | Neural responses to target features outside a search array are enhanced during conjunction but not unique-feature search. |
Q30478011 | Neurophysiological mechanisms involved in language learning in adults |
Q36854990 | Nogo Stimuli Do Not Receive More Attentional Suppression or Response Inhibition than Neutral Stimuli: Evidence from the N2pc, PD, and N2 Components in a Spatial Cueing Paradigm |
Q36002644 | Nonhuman primate event-related potentials indexing covert shifts of attention |
Q41475454 | Object segmentation controls image reconstruction from natural scenes. |
Q50753308 | On the control of visual spatial attention: evidence from human electrophysiology |
Q37416729 | On the origin of event-related potentials indexing covert attentional selection during visual search |
Q36534168 | On the origin of event-related potentials indexing covert attentional selection during visual search: timing of selection by macaque frontal eye field and event-related potentials during pop-out search |
Q36050181 | On the role of selective attention in visual perception |
Q57570229 | Oscillatory gamma-band (30-70 Hz) activity induced by a visual search task in humans |
Q48974088 | Overlap of attention and movement-related activity in lateralized event-related brain potentials |
Q35652765 | Own-race faces capture attention faster than other-race faces: evidence from response time and the N2pc |
Q33605781 | Pattern classification of EEG signals reveals perceptual and attentional states |
Q37336944 | Personal names do not always survive the attentional blink: Behavioral evidence for a flexible locus of selection |
Q48472911 | Predictive distractor context facilitates attentional selection of high, but not intermediate and low, salience targets |
Q41717306 | Predictive value of novel stimuli modifies visual event-related potentials and behavior |
Q36453099 | Preserved Suppression of Salient Irrelevant Stimuli During Visual Search in Age-Associated Memory Impairment |
Q33940473 | Priming of pop-out modulates attentional target selection in visual search: behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. |
Q50479329 | Probability-independent and -dependent ERPs reflecting visual change detection |
Q58808138 | Processing Spatial and Temporal Discontinuities: Electrophysiological Indicators |
Q92246566 | Punishment-related memory-guided attention: Neural dynamics of perceptual modulation |
Q45960383 | Rapid Attentional Selection of Non-native Stimuli despite Perceptual Narrowing. |
Q30397395 | Rapid Context-based Identification of Target Sounds in an Auditory Scene |
Q46073900 | Reduced visual attention in heterogeneous textures is reflected in occipital alpha and theta band activity |
Q36947146 | Response inhibition is linked to emotional devaluation: behavioural and electrophysiological evidence. |
Q58596103 | Retroactive Attentional Shifts Predict Performance in a Working Memory Task: Evidence by Lateralized EEG Patterns |
Q42933599 | Reward changes salience in human vision via the anterior cingulate. |
Q37103828 | Reward priority of visual target singletons modulates event-related potential signatures of attentional selection. |
Q38378911 | Right away: A late, right-lateralized category effect complements an early, left-lateralized category effect in visual search |
Q48807371 | Salience detection and attentional capture |
Q34575115 | Salience-based selection: attentional capture by distractors less salient than the target |
Q34097608 | Saliency changes appearance |
Q47139707 | Scopolamine Reduces Electrophysiological Indices of Distractor Suppression: Evidence from a Contingent Capture Task |
Q37245618 | Searching for something familiar or novel: top-down attentional selection of specific items or object categories |
Q51931913 | Searching for targets within the spatial layout of visual short-term memory. |
Q37318062 | Selection History Modulates Working Memory Capacity |
Q47547008 | Self-face Captures, Holds, and Biases Attention. |
Q49248586 | Sexual Abuse Exposure Alters Early Processing of Emotional Words: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials. |
Q39252188 | Spatial attention across perception and action |
Q30483232 | Spatial attention can bias search in visual short-term memory |
Q51980119 | Spatial attention freezes during the attention blink. |
Q37184671 | Spatial selection of features within perceived and remembered objects |
Q35118995 | Spatiotemporal brain mapping of spatial attention effects on pattern-reversal ERPs |
Q47156102 | Strategic inhibition of distractors with visual working memory contents after involuntary attention capture |
Q51886590 | Subliminally presented and stored objects capture spatial attention. |
Q55076074 | Subtle Distinctions: How Attentional Templates Influence EEG Parameters of Cognitive Control in a Spatial Cuing Paradigm. |
Q50852927 | Suppression of salient objects prevents distraction in visual search. |
Q47771939 | Sustained engagement of attention is associated with increased negative self-referent processing in major depressive disorder |
Q33595213 | Sustained posterior contralateral activity indicates re-entrant target processing in visual change detection: an EEG study |
Q90533900 | Temporal Dynamics of Natural Static Emotional Facial Expressions Decoding: A Study Using Event- and Eye Fixation-Related Potentials |
Q34970414 | Temporal dynamics of visual attention measured with event-related potentials |
Q63252874 | The Allocation of Attention in Change Detection and Change Blindness |
Q38756674 | The COGs (context, object, and goals) in multisensory processing |
Q24645554 | The N2pc component and its links to attention shifts and spatially selective visual processing |
Q34225010 | The N2pc is increased by perceptual learning but is unnecessary for the transfer of learning |
Q37298498 | The Role of Attention in the Binding of Surface Features to Locations |
Q46199760 | The Role of Inhibition in Avoiding Distraction by Salient Stimuli |
Q30375123 | The Temporal Cascade of Neural Processes Underlying Target Detection and Attentional Processing During Auditory Search |
Q91637781 | The Time Course of Target Template Activation Processes during Preparation for Visual Search |
Q33767060 | The attentional blink: past, present, and future of a blind spot in perceptual awareness |
Q42394156 | The attentional selection in visual search within short-term memory representations |
Q37310066 | The beat goes on: rhythmic modulation of cortical potentials by imagined tapping. |
Q34390252 | The binding problem |
Q35765648 | The capacity of audiovisual integration is limited to one item |
Q47837394 | The color of anxiety: neurobehavioral evidence for distraction by perceptually salient stimuli in anxiety |
Q37304523 | The comparison of visual working memory representations with perceptual inputs |
Q36924112 | The construct of attention in schizophrenia |
Q36152112 | The face is more than its parts--brain dynamics of enhanced spatial attention to schematic threat |
Q79802161 | The functional locus of the lateralized readiness potential |
Q38042612 | The importance of being relevant |
Q46810883 | The interaction between pictures and words: evidence from positivity offset and negativity bias |
Q30364695 | The interactions of multisensory integration with endogenous and exogenous attention |
Q38398463 | The interdependence of spatial attention and lexical access as revealed by early asymmetries in occipito-parietal ERP activity. |
Q36984644 | The lasting memory enhancements of retrospective attention |
Q64233484 | The neural correlations of spatial attention and working memory deficits in adults with ADHD |
Q48600788 | The neural site of attention matches the spatial scale of perception. |
Q57155012 | The relationship between attention and consciousness: an expanded taxonomy and implications for 'no-report' paradigms |
Q39949109 | The remains of the trial: goal-determined inter-trial suppression of selective attention. |
Q38847577 | The role of central attention in retrieval from visual short-term memory |
Q34081022 | The role of visual attention in multiple object tracking: Evidence from ERPs |
Q37428875 | The roles of feature-specific task set and bottom-up salience in attentional capture: an ERP study. |
Q34312491 | The scaling of spatial attention in visual search and its modification in healthy aging |
Q36174592 | The temporal dynamics of object processing in visual cortex during the transition from distributed to focused spatial attention. |
Q48446249 | The temporal dynamics of the effects in occipital cortex of visual-spatial selective attention |
Q46639188 | The time course of attentional deployment in contextual cueing |
Q36574260 | The time course of visuo-spatial working memory updating revealed by a retro-cuing paradigm. |
Q92443115 | The time-course of component processes of selective attention |
Q49047387 | The timing of stimulus localisation and the Simon effect: an ERP study |
Q36963810 | Theta Oscillation Reveals the Temporal Involvement of Different Attentional Networks in Contingent Reorienting |
Q37400299 | Tough doughnuts: affect and the modulation of attention |
Q36386409 | Towards the automated localisation of targets in rapid image-sifting by collaborative brain-computer interfaces |
Q33692286 | Value associations of emotional faces can modify the anger superiority effect: behavioral and electrophysiological evidence |
Q93258246 | Value-driven attentional capture is modulated by the contents of working memory: An EEG study |
Q35585439 | Verb aspect and the activation of event knowledge |
Q36675480 | Viewing the dynamics and control of visual attention through the lens of electrophysiology |
Q48451325 | Visual marking for search: behavioral and event-related potential analyses |
Q35237166 | Visual search and the N2pc in children |
Q30944064 | Visual search elicits the electrophysiological marker of visual working memory |
Q34088672 | Visual working memory deficits in patients with Parkinson's disease are due to both reduced storage capacity and impaired ability to filter out irrelevant information |
Q55387902 | Weak surround suppression of the attentional focus characterizes visual selection in the ventral stream in autism. |
Q38444754 | What are task-sets: a single, integrated representation or a collection of multiple control representations? |
Q37694782 | When memory is not enough: electrophysiological evidence for goal-dependent use of working memory representations in guiding visual attention |
Q36673859 | Working memory as internal attention: toward an integrative account of internal and external selection processes |
Q36399223 | Working memory operates over the same representations as attention. |
Search more.