Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect

article

Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect is …
instance of (P31):
scholarly articleQ13442814

External links are
P356DOI10.1037/0022-3514.68.5.804
P5875ResearchGate publication ID232581252

P2093author name stringet al
M. L. Klotz
David L. Breitenbecher
Mark D. Alicke
Tricia J. Yurak
P433issue5
P304page(s)804-825
P577publication date1995-01-01
P1433published inJournal of Personality and Social PsychologyQ1247946
P1476titlePersonal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average effect
P478volume68

Reverse relations

cites work (P2860)
Q46978769A group size effect on personal risk judgments: implications for unrealistic optimism
Q34444230Accuracy of self-evaluation in adults with ADHD: evidence from a driving study
Q36472972An examination of sources of peer-review bias
Q61687786An integrative theory of intergroup contact
Q50998624Are comparative risk judgements consistent across time and events?
Q47990444Assigned experts with competitive goals withhold information in group decision making
Q47818019Behind bars but above the bar: prisoners consider themselves more prosocial than non-prisoners
Q58641957Beliefs About the Effects of Alcohol on the Personality of Oneself and Others
Q41456792Bluffing promotes overconfidence on social networks
Q124053604CEO Overconfidence and Corporate Investment
Q56083268Chapter 1 The Introspection Illusion
Q50910371Comparing You = Comparing Me: Social Comparisons of the Expanded Self.
Q47674856Cultural differences in unrealistic optimism and pessimism: the role of egocentrism and direct versus indirect comparison measures
Q38497992Defining Heroes Through Deductive and Inductive Investigations
Q48591726Effects of anger, guilt, and envy on moral hypocrisy
Q47401650Effects of priming self-construals on self-evaluations: Cultural game player perspective
Q40202241Ego depletion and positive illusions: does the construction of positivity require regulatory resources?
Q28266259Egocentrism, event frequency, and comparative optimism: when what happens frequently is "more likely to happen to me"
Q53993523Enhancement Bias in Descriptions of Self and Others
Q47271609Everybody loves me: self-evaluations and metaperceptions of dating popularity
Q37071723Expectations of benefit in early-phase clinical trials: implications for assessing the adequacy of informed consent
Q50745605Explaining the effect of event valence on unrealistic optimism.
Q60518020Feelings as Information: Moods Influence Judgments and Processing Strategies
Q47270398From Misperception to Social Connection: Correlates and Consequences of Overestimating Others' Social Connectedness
Q33943621From chump to champ: People's appraisals of their earlier and present selves
Q48553874From east to west: accessibility and bias in self-other comparative judgments
Q53983369From wealth to well-being? Money matters, but less than people think
Q50981377Grandparent-grandchild contact and attitudes toward older adults: moderator and mediator effects.
Q92932748Group membership dictates the neural correlates of social optimism biases
Q91684027How and Why Different Forms of Expertise Moderate Anchor Precision in Price Decisions
Q36026133How beliefs about self-creation inflate value in the human brain
Q35589643How self-enhancers adapt well to loss: The mediational role of loneliness and social functioning
Q40258851Humanizing the self: moderators of the attribution of lesser humanness to others
Q37329095I Am a Better Driver Than You Think: Examining Self-Enhancement for Driving Ability
Q47309378Identification and the commitment shift: accounting for gender differences in relationship illusions
Q57833057If it is stored in my memory I will surely retrieve it: anatomy of a metacognitive belief
Q50641888Interhemispheric interaction and egocentrism: the role of handedness in social comparative judgement.
Q47696474Less Evil Than You: Bounded Self-Righteousness in Character Inferences, Emotional Reactions, and Behavioral Extremes
Q56289185Like Goes with Like: The Role of Representativeness in Erroneous and Pseudo-Scientific Beliefs
Q48583164Linking superiority bias in the interpersonal and intergroup domains.
Q48304057Losing our humanity: the self-dehumanizing consequences of social ostracism
Q39728913Motivated expectations of positive feedback in social interactions.
Q38378059Not your average bigot: The better-than-average effect and defensive responding to Implicit Association Test feedback
Q43171961Optimistic bias about H1N1 flu: testing the links between risk communication, optimistic bias, and self-protection behavior
Q51916670Overlapping mental representations of self, ingroup, and outgroup: unraveling self-stereotyping and self-anchoring.
Q34407864Peering into the bias blind spot: people's assessments of bias in themselves and others
Q47617879Perspectives of future health in self and others: The moderating role of culture.
Q53822565RETRACTED: Visual Darkness Reduces Perceived Risk of Contagious-Disease Transmission From Interpersonal Interaction.
Q45158977Rater variables associated with ITER ratings.
Q47426482Resolving the Meat-Paradox: A Motivational Account of Morally Troublesome Behavior and Its Maintenance
Q33624344Reversed better-than-average effect in direct comparisons of nonsocial stimuli depends on the set size.
Q28264824Self-Enhancement: Food for Thought
Q53993470Self-Evaluation: To Thine Own Self Be Good, To Thine Own Self Be Sure, To Thine Own Self Be True, and To Thine Own Self be Better
Q34097402Self-deceived individuals are better at deceiving others
Q35067430Self-esteem modulates the time course of self-positivity bias in explicit self-evaluation
Q40502148Sex differences in pain anchors revisited: further investigation of "most intense" and common pain events.
Q44287257Sexual decision making for the "better than average" college student
Q50720585Sheltering the self from the storm: self-construal abstractness and the stability of self-esteem.
Q52886259Social sampling explains apparent biases in judgments of social environments.
Q48118163Subjective Social Status and Well-Being: The Role of Referent Abstraction
Q46666598The "golden section" and bias in perceptions of social consensus
Q60518048The Availability Heuristic Revisited: Ease of Recall and Content of Recall as Distinct Sources of Information
Q41073191The Better-Than-Average Effect Is Observed Because "Average" Is Often Construed as Below-Median Ability
Q42699797The Illusion of Moral Superiority
Q34263625The Influence of Egocentrism and Focalism on People's Optimism in Competitions: When What Affects Us Equally Affects Me More
Q40935918The SPOT effect: People spontaneously prefer their own theories
Q63540565The Strategic Expression of Personal Belief in a Just World
Q73291723The over-claiming technique: Measuring self-enhancement independent of ability
Q47970305The stakeholders' project in neuropsychological report writing: a survey of neuropsychologists' and referral sources' views of neuropsychological reports
Q50636880The world smiles at me: self-referential positivity bias when interpreting direction of attention.
Q35675588Understanding Collective Discontents: A Psychological Approach to Measuring Zeitgeist
Q56335781Understanding Misunderstanding: Social Psychological Perspectives
Q48020486Understanding the better than average effect: motives (still) matter
Q41455593Unrealistic optimism and the ethics of phase I cancer research
Q50952376Valence, comparison focus and self-positivity biases: does it matter whether people judge positive or negative traits?
Q51183283What do people know about eco-driving?
Q28305767What is the empirical basis for paying for quality in health care?
Q48124348When does event valence affect unrealistic optimism?
Q48297101When focusing on negative and positive attributes of the self elicits more inductive self-judgment
Q46448898When self-affirmations reduce defensiveness: timing is key.