scholarly article | Q13442814 |
P819 | ADS bibcode | 2016PLoSO..1164012T |
P356 | DOI | 10.1371/JOURNAL.PONE.0164012 |
P932 | PMC publication ID | 5082681 |
P698 | PubMed publication ID | 27783704 |
P50 | author | Magnus Johannesson | Q18274567 |
Daniel Västfjäll | Q55128637 | ||
Gustav Tinghög | Q40049610 | ||
P2093 | author name string | David Andersson | |
Lina Koppel | |||
Michael Kirchler | |||
Caroline Bonn | |||
P2860 | cites work | Numeracy and decision making | Q51942176 |
Conflict between intuitive and rational processing: when people behave against their better judgment | Q52217005 | ||
A series of meta-analytic tests of the depletion effect: Self-control does not seem to rely on a limited resource | Q52307234 | ||
Violence restrained: Effects of self-regulation and its depletion on aggression | Q54165930 | ||
Cognitive Reflection and Decision Making | Q56005463 | ||
Dictator games: a meta study | Q56029525 | ||
In a Different Voice: Women's Conceptions of Self and of Morality | Q56048168 | ||
Are social value orientations expressed automatically? Decision making in the dictator game | Q57152080 | ||
The Psychology of Dilemmas and the Philosophy of Morality | Q57837460 | ||
Spontaneous giving and calculated greed | Q59073607 | ||
Gender Differences in Preferences | Q59238300 | ||
Social heuristics and social roles: Intuition favors altruism for women but not for men | Q87364833 | ||
Efficient Kill-Save Ratios Ease Up the Cognitive Demands on Counterintuitive Moral Utilitarianism | Q87659922 | ||
An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment | Q28216269 | ||
Publication bias and the limited strength model of self-control: has the evidence for ego depletion been overestimated? | Q28246023 | ||
Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious | Q28248598 | ||
Ego depletion and the strength model of self-control: a meta-analysis | Q28285796 | ||
The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment | Q30308246 | ||
The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment. | Q30343819 | ||
Cognitive load selectively interferes with utilitarian moral judgment. | Q30482331 | ||
Toward a physiology of dual-process reasoning and judgment: lemonade, willpower, and expensive rule-based analysis | Q34010249 | ||
Gender differences in moral orientation: a meta-analysis | Q34034504 | ||
Human cooperation | Q34357606 | ||
Social heuristics shape intuitive cooperation | Q34416255 | ||
'Utilitarian' judgments in sacrificial moral dilemmas do not reflect impartial concern for the greater good | Q34655356 | ||
Does intuition cause cooperation? | Q35163758 | ||
Prosocial behavior and gender. | Q35409187 | ||
Rethinking fast and slow based on a critique of reaction-time reverse inference | Q35847440 | ||
A Multilab Preregistered Replication of the Ego-Depletion Effect | Q36090553 | ||
Emotion and Deliberative Reasoning in Moral Judgment | Q36208348 | ||
Gender differences in neural mechanisms underlying moral sensitivity | Q37023551 | ||
A meta-analysis of response-time tests of the sequential two-systems model of moral judgment | Q39061149 | ||
Commentary: Fairness is intuitive. | Q41158939 | ||
Models of morality | Q41958384 | ||
Intuition and cooperation reconsidered | Q44491155 | ||
Deontological and utilitarian inclinations in moral decision making: A process dissociation approach | Q45372784 | ||
Mortality salience and morality: thinking about death makes people less utilitarian. | Q46000994 | ||
Time and moral judgment | Q48151253 | ||
Who shalt not kill? Individual differences in working memory capacity, executive control, and moral judgment | Q48336258 | ||
Moral dilemmas and moral rules | Q48475199 | ||
Reflection and reasoning in moral judgment | Q48554957 | ||
Stress alters personal moral decision making | Q48596248 | ||
P275 | copyright license | Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International | Q20007257 |
P6216 | copyright status | copyrighted | Q50423863 |
P433 | issue | 10 | |
P407 | language of work or name | English | Q1860 |
P921 | main subject | decision making | Q1331926 |
moral decision-making | Q106589010 | ||
altruistic behaviour | Q131373087 | ||
P304 | page(s) | e0164012 | |
P577 | publication date | 2016-10-26 | |
P1433 | published in | PLOS One | Q564954 |
P1476 | title | Intuition and Moral Decision-Making - The Effect of Time Pressure and Cognitive Load on Moral Judgment and Altruistic Behavior | |
P478 | volume | 11 |
Q90663454 | Bullshit-sensitivity predicts prosocial behavior |
Q33571634 | Commentary: Empathy and its discontents. |
Q45950265 | Does Short-Term Hunger Increase Trust and Trustworthiness in a High Trust Society? |
Q47142656 | Extending the Cooperative Phenotype: Assessing the Stability of Cooperation across Countries. |
Q90610808 | How does the method change what we measure? Comparing virtual reality and text-based surveys for the assessment of moral decisions in traffic dilemmas |
Q89959740 | Medical Students' Attitude Toward Organ Donation in a Single Medical University |
Q90443127 | No Correlation Between Ethical Judgment in Trolley Dilemmas and Vaccine Scenarios for Nurse Specialist Students |
Q89620415 | Opportunity Cost in Monetary Donation Decisions to Non-identified and Identified Victims |
Q104748252 | RETRACTED: Beyond moral dilemmas: The role of reasoning in five categories of utilitarian judgment |
Q41035113 | Spot on for liars! How public scrutiny influences ethical behavior |
Q102152214 | Using quantitative trait in adults with ADHD to test predictions of dual-process theory |
Search more.