scholarly article | Q13442814 |
P50 | author | Daniel Lakens | Q57810589 |
P2093 | author name string | Alexander J Etz | |
P2860 | cites work | Why most published research findings are false | Q21092395 |
"Positive" results increase down the Hierarchy of the Sciences | Q21136404 | ||
An exploratory test for an excess of significant findings | Q24273224 | ||
A Bayesian Perspective on the Reproducibility Project: Psychology | Q24288635 | ||
The N-pact factor: evaluating the quality of empirical journals with respect to sample size and statistical power | Q24288678 | ||
Researchers' Intuitions About Power in Psychological Research. | Q27324368 | ||
Assessing the probability that a positive report is false: an approach for molecular epidemiology studies | Q29616285 | ||
The illusion of nonmediation in telecommunication: voice intensity biases distance judgments to a communication partner. | Q39031996 | ||
Continuously Cumulating Meta-Analysis and Replicability | Q40738647 | ||
The frequency of excess success for articles in Psychological Science | Q43419573 | ||
P-curve: A key to the file-drawer | Q51186445 | ||
The ironic effect of significant results on the credibility of multiple-study articles | Q51329567 | ||
The propagation of self-control: Self-control in one domain simultaneously improves self-control in other domains | Q53169119 | ||
Belief in the law of small numbers | Q55887932 | ||
P433 | issue | 8 | |
P304 | page(s) | 875-881 | |
P577 | publication date | 2017-05-05 | |
P1433 | published in | Social Psychological and Personality Science | Q15716265 |
P1476 | title | Too True to be Bad: When Sets of Studies With Significant and Nonsignificant Findings Are Probably True | |
P478 | volume | 8 |
Q56888011 | "Eavesdropping on Happiness" Revisited: A Pooled, Multisample Replication of the Association Between Life Satisfaction and Observed Daily Conversation Quantity and Quality |
Q57906431 | A role for meta-analysis in hydrology |
Q95646786 | Addressing selective reporting of experiments through predefined exclusion criteria |
Q49483588 | Agency Beliefs Over Time and Across Cultures: Free Will Beliefs Predict Higher Job Satisfaction |
Q47134251 | Feeling younger and identifying with older adults: Testing two routes to maintaining well-being in the face of age discrimination. |
Q91917481 | Having pity on our victims to save ourselves: Compassion reduces self-critical emotions and self-blame about past harmful behavior among those who highly identify with their past self |
Q92902636 | Improving Psychological Science through Transparency and Openness: An Overview |
Q55715082 | Open Science Is Liberating and Can Foster Creativity. |
Q64216448 | Peer-Review Guidelines Promoting Replicability and Transparency in Psychological Science |
Q55251359 | Recognizing Emily and Latisha: Inconsistent Effects of Name Stereotypicality on the Other-Race Effect. |
Q102075341 | Revisiting potential associations between brain morphology, fear acquisition and extinction through new data and a literature review |
Q57031163 | Reward-associated distractors can harm cognitive performance |
Q64946904 | So Gross and Yet so Far Away: Psychological Distance Moderates the Effect of Disgust on Moral Judgment. |
Q37230242 | Tagging motor memories with transcranial direct current stimulation allows later artificially-controlled retrieval. |
Q64923471 | The Longitudinal Association between Self-esteem and Depressive Symptoms in Adolescents: Separating between-person effects from within-person effects. |
Q48105847 | When Action-Inaction Framing Leads to Higher Escalation of Commitment: A New Inaction-Effect Perspective on the Sunk-Cost Fallacy |
Q89146356 | Would disfluency by any other name still be disfluent? Examining the disfluency effect with cursive handwriting |
Search more.