scholarly article | Q13442814 |
P6179 | Dimensions Publication ID | 1041343171 |
P356 | DOI | 10.3758/S13423-012-0322-Y |
P8608 | Fatcat ID | release_sb763bgiojbjlka7poa44fbrwi |
P953 | full work available at URL | http://link.springer.com/article/10.3758/s13423-012-0322-y/fulltext.html |
http://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.3758/s13423-012-0322-y | ||
http://link.springer.com/content/pdf/10.3758/s13423-012-0322-y.pdf | ||
P3181 | OpenCitations bibliographic resource ID | 1120808 |
P698 | PubMed publication ID | 23055145 |
P50 | author | Gregory Francis | Q61770415 |
P2860 | cites work | Journal of Scientific Exploration | Q6295889 |
Journal of the American Statistical Association | Q4181923 | ||
Altered vision near the hands | Q81540987 | ||
Reaching out with the imagination | Q83365451 | ||
Journal of Articles in Support of the Null Hypothesis | Q102396570 | ||
Out of India, thrice: diversification of Asian forest scorpions reveals three colonizations of Southeast Asia | Q104501843 | ||
Of Beauty, Sex and Power | Q114106131 | ||
An exploratory test for an excess of significant findings | Q24273224 | ||
Too good to be true: Publication bias in two prominent studies from experimental psychology | Q24273228 | ||
False-Positive Psychology: Undisclosed Flexibility in Data Collection and Analysis Allows Presenting Anything as Significant | Q24273231 | ||
Why Most Discovered True Associations Are Inflated | Q24273233 | ||
Operating characteristics of a rank correlation test for publication bias | Q27860653 | ||
The (mis)reporting of statistical results in psychology journals | Q28923488 | ||
Publication and related bias in meta-analysis: power of statistical tests and prevalence in the literature | Q29614233 | ||
Bayesian data analysis | Q30987243 | ||
Measuring the Prevalence of Questionable Research Practices With Incentives for Truth Telling | Q34031507 | ||
Feeling the future: Experimental evidence for anomalous retroactive influences on cognition and affect | Q34161614 | ||
Unpublished results hide the decline effect | Q34166781 | ||
The bystander-effect: a meta-analytic review on bystander intervention in dangerous and non-dangerous emergencies | Q34181684 | ||
Washing away your sins: threatened morality and physical cleansing | Q34564552 | ||
The same old New Look: Publication bias in a study of wishful seeing. | Q36362140 | ||
Bayesian Versus Orthodox Statistics: Which Side Are You On? | Q38546092 | ||
A Bayes factor meta-analysis of Bem's ESP claim | Q39749980 | ||
The ironic effect of significant results on the credibility of multiple-study articles | Q51329567 | ||
Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. | Q51848247 | ||
A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values | Q51898263 | ||
How reliable are scientific studies? | Q53074668 | ||
Publication bias in “Red, rank, and romance in women viewing men,” by Elliot et al. (2010) | Q55057014 | ||
Consequences of prejudice against the null hypothesis | Q56699993 | ||
SNOOP:a program for demonstrating the consequences of premature and repeated null hypothesis testing | Q79831522 | ||
P433 | issue | 6 | |
P407 | language of work or name | English | Q1860 |
P921 | main subject | bias | Q742736 |
publication bias | Q919364 | ||
reproducibility | Q1425625 | ||
P304 | page(s) | 975-991 | |
P577 | publication date | 2012-10-04 | |
2012-12-01 | |||
P1433 | published in | Psychonomic Bulletin and Review | Q15763410 |
P1476 | title | Publication bias and the failure of replication in experimental psychology | |
P478 | volume | 19 |
Q40781168 | A Bayesian approach to mitigation of publication bias |
Q28656062 | A call to honesty: extending religious priming of moral behavior to Middle Eastern Muslims |
Q39458805 | A meta-analysis of the survival-processing advantage in memory |
Q38253636 | Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs |
Q57584181 | Construção de uma escala para avaliar a qualidade metodológica de revisões sistemáticas |
Q57124761 | Criminalization of scientific misconduct |
Q45894410 | Decisive evidence on a smaller-than-you-think phenomenon: revisiting the "1-in-X" effect on subjective medical probabilities |
Q80185758 | Detecting Evidential Value and p-Hacking With the p-Curve Tool: A Word of Caution |
Q43376606 | Does chronic nicotine consumption influence visual backward masking in schizophrenia and schizotypy? |
Q34514193 | Efficacy of a web-based intelligent tutoring system for communicating genetic risk of breast cancer: a fuzzy-trace theory approach |
Q36164362 | Equivalent statistics and data interpretation. |
Q47802892 | Evidence-based science policy for mental health in a post-truth era. |
Q47097509 | Examining publication bias-a simulation-based evaluation of statistical tests on publication bias |
Q28650336 | Excess success for psychology articles in the journal science |
Q90090096 | How Many Participants Do We Have to Include in Properly Powered Experiments? A Tutorial of Power Analysis with Reference Tables |
Q90398777 | Hypothesis Testing in the Real World |
Q99544121 | Meta Research: Questionable research practices may have little effect on replicability |
Q28607670 | Meta-analyses are no substitute for registered replications: a skeptical perspective on religious priming |
Q52659599 | Metastudies for robust tests of theory. |
Q39494412 | Modeling stimulus variation in three common implicit attitude tasks |
Q24288901 | Negativland - a home for all findings in psychology |
Q28591178 | Preventing the ends from justifying the means: withholding results to address publication bias in peer-review |
Q24658632 | Psychology, replication & beyond |
Q58720372 | Published estimates of group differences in multisensory integration are inflated |
Q40925702 | Replicating studies in which samples of participants respond to samples of stimuli |
Q43625418 | Replication in Psychological Science |
Q39510357 | Researchers' choice of the number and range of levels in experiments affects the resultant variance-accounted-for effect size. |
Q49909483 | Response preparation, response selection difficulty, and response-outcome learning. |
Q58175172 | Restoring confidence in psychological science findings: A call for direct replication studies |
Q37474784 | Statistical learning theory for high dimensional prediction: Application to criterion-keyed scale development |
Q37652223 | Strong Stackelberg reasoning in symmetric games: An experimental replication and extension. |
Q64087193 | The Differential Effects of Good Luck Belief on Cognitive Performance in Boys and Girls |
Q38783347 | The Influence of Attachment Styles on Sexual Communication Behavior |
Q43419573 | The frequency of excess success for articles in Psychological Science |
Q48879269 | The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: myth or reality? |
Q60922301 | The quest for an optimal alpha |
Q57248833 | The replication paradox: Combining studies can decrease accuracy of effect size estimates |
Q37613857 | Toward a neural basis for social behavior |
Q38573256 | Visual category learning |
Q34860724 | Visual statistical learning is not reliably modulated by selective attention to isolated events |
Q35180471 | What explains consciousness? Or…What consciousness explains? |
Q23307991 | Why publishing everything is more effective than selective publishing of statistically significant results |
Search more.