scholarly article | Q13442814 |
P50 | author | John Ioannidis | Q6251482 |
Marcus R. Munafò | Q21094872 | ||
Paolo Fusar-Poli | Q21094931 | ||
Brian Nosek | Q22096791 | ||
Sean P. David | Q47818322 | ||
P2860 | cites work | Why current publication practices may distort science | Q21563409 |
Drug development: Raise standards for preclinical cancer research | Q22348097 | ||
Selective Publication of Antidepressant Trials and Its Influence on Apparent Efficacy | Q23791313 | ||
Negative results are disappearing from most disciplines and countries | Q24273200 | ||
The file drawer problem and tolerance for null results | Q24273204 | ||
Publication bias in reports of animal stroke studies leads to major overstatement of efficacy | Q24273209 | ||
Excess Significance Bias in the Literature on Brain Volume Abnormalities | Q24273222 | ||
An exploratory test for an excess of significant findings | Q24273224 | ||
False-Positive Psychology: Undisclosed Flexibility in Data Collection and Analysis Allows Presenting Anything as Significant | Q24273231 | ||
Effect of the Statistical Significance of Results on the Time to Completion and Publication of Randomized Efficacy Trials | Q74121809 | ||
Developmental biology. Fraud investigation clouds paper on early cell fate | Q79397288 | ||
A bound for publication bias based on the fraction of unpublished studies | Q79801825 | ||
A robust P-value for treatment effect in meta-analysis with publication bias | Q81003504 | ||
Scientific misconduct. Fraud charges cast doubt on claims of DNA damage from cell phone fields | Q81893643 | ||
Genomewide association studies--illuminating biologic pathways | Q83658310 | ||
Science or Art? How Aesthetic Standards Grease the Way Through the Publication Bottleneck but Undermine Science | Q24273232 | ||
Why Most Discovered True Associations Are Inflated | Q24273233 | ||
Scientific Utopia: II. Restructuring Incentives and Practices to Promote Truth Over Publishability | Q24273236 | ||
Quantifying selective reporting and the Proteus phenomenon for multiple datasets with similar bias | Q24288719 | ||
Practices and impact of primary outcome adjustment in randomized controlled trials: meta-epidemiologic study | Q24289045 | ||
US studies may overestimate effect sizes in softer research | Q24289257 | ||
The case of the misleading funnel plot | Q24676535 | ||
Making Prospective Registration of Observational Research a Reality | Q26827609 | ||
Systematic review of the empirical evidence of study publication bias and outcome reporting bias - an updated review | Q26851424 | ||
Publication bias in laboratory animal research: a survey on magnitude, drivers, consequences and potential solutions | Q27323022 | ||
Trim and fill: A simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis | Q27860511 | ||
Outcome reporting in industry-sponsored trials of gabapentin for off-label use | Q28264491 | ||
An Open, Large-Scale, Collaborative Effort to Estimate the Reproducibility of Psychological Science | Q28264997 | ||
World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects | Q28300546 | ||
Publication bias in antipsychotic trials: an analysis of efficacy comparing the published literature to the US Food and Drug Administration database | Q28481584 | ||
The power of meta-analysis in genome-wide association studies | Q28658972 | ||
Novel methods to deal with publication biases: secondary analysis of antidepressant trials in the FDA trial registry database and related journal publications | Q28752005 | ||
Investigating Variation in Replicability | Q28969609 | ||
Power failure: why small sample size undermines the reliability of neuroscience | Q29547474 | ||
Believe it or not: how much can we rely on published data on potential drug targets? | Q29547529 | ||
A modified test for small-study effects in meta-analyses of controlled trials with binary endpoints | Q29614902 | ||
Replication validity of genetic association studies | Q29615456 | ||
Comparison of two methods to detect publication bias in meta-analysis | Q29617904 | ||
Systematic review of the empirical evidence of study publication bias and outcome reporting bias | Q29619094 | ||
Recommendations for examining and interpreting funnel plot asymmetry in meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials | Q29620299 | ||
Rewarding Replications: A Sure and Simple Way to Improve Psychological Science. | Q30376616 | ||
Meta-analysis of sex differences in rodent models of learning and memory: a review of behavioral and biological data | Q30980131 | ||
Early extreme contradictory estimates may appear in published research: the Proteus phenomenon in molecular genetics research and randomized trials | Q31170846 | ||
Testing for the presence of positive-outcome bias in peer review: a randomized controlled trial | Q34024803 | ||
Measuring the Prevalence of Questionable Research Practices With Incentives for Truth Telling | Q34031507 | ||
Replications in Psychology Research: How Often Do They Really Occur? | Q34044437 | ||
Assessing the potential for bias in meta-analysis due to selective reporting of subgroup analyses within studies | Q34107060 | ||
The case for motivated reasoning | Q34281854 | ||
The secret lives of experiments: methods reporting in the fMRI literature | Q34337694 | ||
The importance of potential studies that have not existed and registration of observational data sets | Q34370321 | ||
Inappropriate fiddling with statistical analyses to obtain a desirable p-value: tests to detect its presence in published literature | Q34441349 | ||
Teaching Replication | Q34484647 | ||
Harnessing the Undiscovered Resource of Student Research Projects. | Q34484653 | ||
An Agenda for Purely Confirmatory Research | Q34484661 | ||
Evidence for the efficacy of NXY-059 in experimental focal cerebral ischaemia is confounded by study quality | Q34797518 | ||
Potential reporting bias in fMRI studies of the brain | Q34921995 | ||
A proposed method of bias adjustment for meta-analyses of published observational studies. | Q35137942 | ||
Guidelines for reporting an fMRI study | Q36527046 | ||
The impact of pharmaceutical company funding on results of randomized trials of nicotine replacement therapy for smoking cessation: a meta-analysis | Q36817282 | ||
How can we improve the pre-clinical development of drugs for stroke? | Q36926526 | ||
The impact of outcome reporting bias in randomised controlled trials on a cohort of systematic reviews | Q37692721 | ||
Efficacy of cognitive-behavioural therapy and other psychological treatments for adult depression: meta-analytic study of publication bias | Q37701131 | ||
Commentary: Adjusting for bias: a user's guide to performing plastic surgery on meta-analyses of observational studies | Q37828304 | ||
Dissecting the genetic architecture of human personality | Q37915029 | ||
Why are psychiatric imaging methods clinically unreliable? Conclusions and practical guidelines for authors, editors and reviewers | Q38039519 | ||
Perceived information gain from randomized trials correlates with publication in high-impact factor journals | Q38041812 | ||
Undue industry influences that distort healthcare research, strategy, expenditure and practice: a review | Q38092146 | ||
Meta-analysis methods for genome-wide association studies and beyond | Q38105261 | ||
Retractions in the scientific literature: do authors deliberately commit research fraud? | Q38426397 | ||
Study factors influencing ventricular enlargement in schizophrenia: A 20year follow-up meta-analysis | Q41845739 | ||
Publication Bias in Neuroimaging Research: Implications for Meta-Analyses | Q42136639 | ||
A peculiar prevalence of p values just below .05. | Q43447558 | ||
Evidence of reporting biases in voxel-based morphometry (VBM) studies of psychiatric and neurological disorders | Q43487463 | ||
Comparison of psychotherapies for adult depression to pill placebo control groups: a meta-analysis | Q43927027 | ||
Business not as usual | Q44016260 | ||
Detecting and adjusting for small-study effects in meta-analysis | Q44984775 | ||
Extrapolating from Animals to Humans | Q46826177 | ||
Scientific misconduct. Psychologist accused of fraud on 'astonishing scale'. | Q48432266 | ||
The Psychology of Replication and Replication in Psychology. | Q50930790 | ||
P-curve: A key to the file-drawer | Q51186445 | ||
Reliability and replicability of genetic association studies | Q52295100 | ||
Special Issue ofSocial Psychologyon “Replications of Important Results in Social Psychology” | Q53464594 | ||
Estimating Effect Size Under Publication Bias: Small Sample Properties and Robustness of a Random Effects Selection Model | Q56289277 | ||
Shall we really do it again? The powerful concept of replication is neglected in the social sciences | Q56681049 | ||
Consequences of prejudice against the null hypothesis | Q56699993 | ||
Registered Reports: A new publishing initiative at Cortex | Q56854673 | ||
NIH mulls rules for validating key results | Q59065516 | ||
Normative Dissonance in Science: Results from a National Survey of U.S. Scientists | Q63248025 | ||
P433 | issue | 5 | |
P407 | language of work or name | English | Q1860 |
P921 | main subject | prevention | Q1717246 |
bias | Q742736 | ||
P304 | page(s) | 235-241 | |
P577 | publication date | 2014-03-18 | |
P13046 | publication type of scholarly work | review article | Q7318358 |
P1433 | published in | Trends in Cognitive Sciences | Q7838299 |
P1476 | title | Publication and other reporting biases in cognitive sciences: detection, prevalence, and prevention | |
P478 | volume | 18 |
Q44566203 | "Me & my brain": exposing neuroscience's closet dualism |
Q57896763 | (When) Is Science Reporting Ethical? The Case for Recognizing Shared Epistemic Responsibility in Science Journalism |
Q26784491 | A Meta-Analytic Review of Stand-Alone Interventions to Improve Body Image |
Q27826377 | A Practical Guide for Improving Transparency and Reproducibility in Neuroimaging Research |
Q42699271 | A Systems Approach to Understanding and Improving Research Integrity. |
Q24273356 | A critical review of the neuroimaging literature on synesthesia |
Q22914736 | A practical guide for improving transparency and reproducibility in neuroimaging research |
Q24273251 | A surge of p-values between 0.041 and 0.049 in recent decades (but negative results are increasing rapidly too) |
Q31134667 | A systematic review and secondary data analysis of the interactions between the serotonin transporter 5-HTTLPR polymorphism and environmental and psychological factors in eating disorders |
Q39201225 | Altered Brain Activity in Unipolar Depression Revisited: Meta-analyses of Neuroimaging Studies |
Q24273259 | An open investigation of the reproducibility of cancer biology research |
Q52606555 | Analytical Transparency and Reproducibility in Human Neuroimaging Studies |
Q57152847 | Are Whites and minorities more similar than different? Testing the cultural similarities hypothesis on psychopathology with a second-order meta-analysis |
Q64967118 | Assessing for interaction between APOE ε4, sex, and lifestyle on cognitive abilities. |
Q92022963 | Associations between work satisfaction, engagement and 7-day patient mortality: a cross-sectional survey |
Q58710398 | Biases Inherent in Studies of Coffee Consumption in Early Pregnancy and the Risks of Subsequent Events |
Q89612701 | Bilingualism Is Associated with a Delayed Onset of Dementia but Not with a Lower Risk of Developing it: a Systematic Review with Meta-Analyses |
Q39949197 | Brain structure in autism: a voxel-based morphometry analysis of the Autism Brain Imaging Database Exchange (ABIDE). |
Q90641595 | CONSORT-SPI 2018 Explanation and Elaboration: guidance for reporting social and psychological intervention trials |
Q39828901 | Can We Trust Positive Findings of Intervention Research? The Role of Conflict of Interest |
Q24273240 | Candidate gene-environment interaction research: reflections and recommendations |
Q59263485 | Citizenship, Social Media, and Big Data |
Q38728703 | Cognitive Characterization of Schizophrenia Risk Variants Involved in Synaptic Transmission: Evidence of CACNA1C's Role in Working Memory |
Q51231489 | Commentary on Thrul et al. (2017): A welcome step towards a more context-aware addiction science |
Q24289482 | Coping with the Conflict-of-Interest Pandemic by Listening to and Doubting Everyone, Including Yourself |
Q47762525 | Creativity and Mood Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis |
Q63365564 | Data science for the scientific life cycle |
Q30368900 | Dispositional negativity: An integrative psychological and neurobiological perspective |
Q55418652 | Dissociating frontoparietal brain networks with neuroadaptive Bayesian optimization. |
Q38604922 | Does Aerobic Exercise Influence Intrinsic Brain Activity? An Aerobic Exercise Intervention among Healthy Old Adults |
Q38637819 | Does Oxytocin Increase Trust in Humans? A Critical Review of Research |
Q31031716 | ENIGMA and the individual: Predicting factors that affect the brain in 35 countries worldwide |
Q92966981 | Environmental risk factors and biomarkers for autism spectrum disorder: an umbrella review of the evidence |
Q57278037 | Evaluation of the causal effects between subjective wellbeing and cardiometabolic health: mendelian randomisation study |
Q90566738 | Evidence Relating Health Care Provider Burnout and Quality of Care: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis |
Q38950205 | Exercise for depression in older adults: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials adjusting for publication bias |
Q28596536 | Failure to Replicate: Sound the Alarm |
Q38893848 | Forty years of structural imaging in psychosis: promises and truth |
Q28610632 | From Big Data to Knowledge in the Social Sciences |
Q38654618 | From efficacy to effectiveness to broad implementation: Evolution of the Body Project |
Q57561457 | Gender Differences in Spatial Ability: Implications for STEM Education and Approaches to Reducing the Gender Gap for Parents and Educators |
Q38808607 | Genetic influences on adolescent behavior |
Q98946620 | High impact nutrition and dietetics journals' use of publication procedures to increase research transparency |
Q92574472 | How technical progress reshaped behavioral neuroendocrinology during the last 50 years… and some methodological remarks |
Q47611691 | Human fear acquisition deficits in relation to genetic variants of the corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor 1 and the serotonin transporter--revisited |
Q35643303 | I just ran a thousand analyses: benefits of multiple testing in understanding equivocal evidence on gene-environment interactions |
Q89998477 | Impairments of large-scale functional networks in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity |
Q58762140 | Indirect (implicit) and direct (explicit) self-esteem measures are virtually unrelated: A meta-analysis of the initial preference task |
Q28597997 | Internal conceptual replications do not increase independent replication success |
Q48828993 | Intranasal administration of oxytocin modulates behavioral and amygdala responses to infant crying in females with insecure attachment representations |
Q38604888 | Investigating Direct Links between Depression, Emotional Control, and Physical Punishment with Adolescent Drive for Thinness and Bulimic Behaviors, Including Possible Moderation by the Serotonin Transporter 5-HTTLPR Polymorphism |
Q33867256 | Lack of Associations between Female Hormone Levels and Visuospatial Working Memory, Divided Attention and Cognitive Bias across Two Consecutive Menstrual Cycles. |
Q28601122 | Less Is More: Psychologists Can Learn More by Studying Fewer People |
Q64241961 | Level and Prevalence of Spin in Published Cardiovascular Randomized Clinical Trial Reports With Statistically Nonsignificant Primary Outcomes: A Systematic Review |
Q24273343 | Linear mixed-effects models for within-participant psychology experiments: an introductory tutorial and free, graphical user interface (LMMgui) |
Q95270069 | Mapping brain asymmetry in health and disease through the ENIGMA consortium |
Q90031178 | Meta-analytic review of dissonance-based eating disorder prevention programs: Intervention, participant, and facilitator features that predict larger effects |
Q30827798 | Methods and considerations for longitudinal structural brain imaging analysis across development. |
Q87419858 | Most psychotherapies do not really work, but those that might work should be assessed in biased studies |
Q90732074 | Neural Correlates of Self-Construal Priming in the Ultimatum Game |
Q35790461 | Neural changes with attention bias modification for anxiety: a randomized trial |
Q36250475 | Neural correlates of executive attention in adults born very preterm |
Q36407551 | Neural correlates of masked and unmasked face emotion processing in youth with severe mood dysregulation |
Q39144586 | Neuroscience in gambling policy and treatment: an interdisciplinary perspective |
Q30395750 | New Issues for New Methods: Ethical and Editorial Challenges for an Experimental Philosophy |
Q61822094 | Nudging transparent behavioural science and policy. |
Q28598096 | Outcome Reporting Bias in Government-Sponsored Policy Evaluations: A Qualitative Content Analysis of 13 Studies |
Q52582065 | Potential Reporting Bias in Neuroimaging Studies of Sex Differences |
Q91722325 | Predicting the replicability of social science lab experiments |
Q24273253 | Problems in using p-curve analysis and text-mining to detect rate of p-hacking and evidential value |
Q29994537 | Progress toward openness, transparency, and reproducibility in cognitive neuroscience |
Q37740823 | Putative sex-specific human pheromones do not affect gender perception, attractiveness ratings or unfaithfulness judgements of opposite sex faces |
Q36309082 | Questionable research practices among italian research psychologists |
Q90701551 | Remapping in Cerebral and Cerebellar Cortices Is Not Restricted by Somatotopy |
Q28597442 | Replication Validity of Initial Association Studies: A Comparison between Psychiatry, Neurology and Four Somatic Diseases |
Q90641530 | Reporting randomised trials of social and psychological interventions: the CONSORT-SPI 2018 Extension |
Q98720330 | Reproducibility in the absence of selective reporting: An illustration from large-scale brain asymmetry research |
Q28608410 | SCIENTIFIC STANDARDS. Promoting an open research culture |
Q80185753 | Seven Easy Steps to Open Science: An Annotated Reading List |
Q42173299 | Six Questions for the Resource Model of Control (and Some Answers). |
Q47626140 | Spillover effects in epidemiology: parameters, study designs and methodological considerations. |
Q37588586 | Subtle Scientific Fallacies Undermine the Validity of Neuroendocrinological Research: Do Not Draw Premature Conclusions on the Role of Female Sex Hormones |
Q47406336 | Ten simple rules for neuroimaging meta-analysis |
Q58598222 | The Correlation Between Neonatal Intensive Care Unit Safety Culture and Quality of Care |
Q89467375 | The Language of Science |
Q39235470 | The Origins of Cognitive Deficits in Victimized Children: Implications for Neuroscientists and Clinicians |
Q48009927 | The brain disease model of addiction: is it supported by the evidence and has it delivered on its promises? |
Q55356134 | The impact of sample size on the reproducibility of voxel-based lesion-deficit mappings. |
Q28081897 | The neurobiology of emotion-cognition interactions: fundamental questions and strategies for future research |
Q92010242 | The role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex in automatic formation of impression and reflected impression |
Q28651356 | The search for human pheromones: the lost decades and the necessity of returning to first principles |
Q30919230 | Thinking About Data, Research Methods, and Statistical Analyses: Commentary on Sijtsma's (2014) "Playing with Data". |
Q64068053 | Thought-based interaction: Same data, same methods, different results? |
Q24288977 | Top 10 Replicated Findings From Behavioral Genetics |
Q36547724 | Underpowered samples, false negatives, and unconscious learning. |
Q24289249 | Using prediction markets to estimate the reproducibility of scientific research |
Q93135625 | Virtual reality exposure therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD): a meta-analysis |
Q47547157 | What causes psychosis? An umbrella review of risk and protective factors |
Q35408386 | When more data steer us wrong: replications with the wrong dependent measure perpetuate erroneous conclusions |
Search more.